|
A free membership is required to access uploaded content. Login or Register.
Social Psychology, 7/E - Chapter 4
|
Uploaded: 7 years ago
Category: Psychology and Mental Health
Type: Lecture Notes
Rating:
N/A
|
Filename: aronson_7e_ch04_perception.ppt
(892.5 kB)
Page Count: 73
Credit Cost: 2
Views: 119
Last Download: N/A
|
Description
Elliot Aronson, University of California, Santa Cruz Timothy D. Wilson, University of Virginia Robin M. Akert, Wellesley College
Transcript
Social Psychology
Elliot Aronson
University of California, Santa Cruz
Timothy D. Wilson
University of Virginia
Robin M. Akert
Wellesley College
slides prepared by
Travis Langley
Henderson State University
7th edition
Chapter 4
Social Perception: How We Come to Understand Other People
“Things are seldom as they seem. Skim milk masquerades as cream.”
– W. S. Gilbert
Other people are not easy to figure out.
Why are they the way they are?
Why do they do what they do?
We all have a fundamental fascination with explaining other people’s behavior, but all we have to go on is observable behavior:
What people do
What they say
Facial expressions
Gestures
Tone of voice
We can’t know, truly and completely, who they are and what they mean.
Instead, we rely on our impressions and personal theories, putting them together as well as we can, hoping they will lead to reasonably accurate and useful conclusions.
Social Perception
The study of how we form impressions of and make inferences about other people.
Social Perception
Nonverbal Behavior
What do we know about people when we first meet them?
We know what we can see and hear, and even though we know we should not judge a book by its cover, this kind of easily observable information is crucial to our first impression.
With no words at all, we can communicate volumes.
Nonverbal Behavior
Nonverbal Communication
The way in which people communicate, intentionally or unintentionally, without words.
Nonverbal cues include:
facial expressions
tone of voice
gestures
body position/movement
the use of touch
gaze
Nonverbal Behavior
We have a special kind of brain cell called mirror neurons.
These neurons respond when we perform an action and when we see someone else perform the same action.
Mirror neurons appear to be the basis of our ability to feel empathy.
For example, when we see someone crying, these mirror neurons fire automatically and involuntarily, just as if we were crying ourselves.
Nonverbal Behavior
Nonverbal cues serve many functions in communication.
You can express “I’m angry” by narrowing your eyes, lowering your eyebrows, and setting your mouth in a thin, straight line.
You can convey the attitude “I like you” with smiles and extended eye contact.
And you communicate your personality traits, like being an extrovert, with broad gestures and frequent changes in voice pitch and inflection.
Nonverbal Behavior
Some nonverbal cues actually contradict the spoken words.
Communicating sarcasm is the classic example of verbal-nonverbal contradiction.
Think about how you’d say “I’m so happy for you” sarcastically.
Facial Expressions of Emotion
Are facial expressions of emotion universal?
The answer is yes, for the six major emotional expressions: anger, happiness, surprise, fear, disgust, and sadness.
All humans encode or express these emotions in the same way, and all humans can decode or interpret them with equal accuracy.
Facial Expressions of Emotion
Paul Ekman and others have conducted numerous studies indicating that the ability to interpret at least the six major emotions is cross-cultural—part of being human and not a product of people’s cultural experience.
Facial Expressions of Emotion
Other emotions such as guilt, shame, embarrassment, and pride occur later in human development and show less universality.
These latter emotions are closely tied to social interaction.
Facial Expressions of Emotion
Decoding facial expressions accurately is more complicated than we have indicated, for three reasons.
Affect blends occur when one part of the face registers one emotion and another part, a different emotion.
At times people try to appear less emotional than they are so that no one will know how they really feel.
A third reason why decoding facial expressions can be inaccurate has to do with culture.
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication
Display rules are particular to each culture and dictate what kinds of emotional expressions people are supposed to show.
Examples of display rule differences:
American cultural norms discourage emotional displays in men, such as grief or crying, but allow the facial display of such emotions in women.
Japanese women will often hide a wide smile behind their hands, whereas Western women are allowed—indeed, encouraged—to smile broadly and often.
Japanese norms lead people to cover up negative facial expressions with smiles and laughter and to display fewer facial expressions in general than is true in the West.
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication
Members of American culture become suspicious when a person doesn’t “look them in the eye” while speaking, and they find talking to someone who is wearing dark sunglasses quite disconcerting.
Cultures vary greatly in what is considered normative use of personal space. Most Americans like to have a bubble of open space, a few feet in radius, surrounding them; in comparison, in some other cultures, strangers think nothing of standing right next to each other, to the point of touching.
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication
The important point about emblems is that they are not universal.
Each culture has devised its own emblems, and these need not be understandable to people from other cultures.
Emblems
Nonverbal gestures that have well-understood definitions within a given culture; they usually have direct verbal translations, like the “OK” sign.
President George H. W. Bush once used the “V for victory” sign, but he did it backward—the palm of his hand was facing him instead of the audience. Unfortunately, he flashed this gesture to a large crowd in Australia—and in Australia, this emblem is the equivalent of “flipping the bird”!
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication
Multichannel Nonverbal Communication
Because nonverbal information is diffused across these many channels, we can often rely on one channel to understand what is going on.
This increases our ability to make accurate judgments about others.
Except for certain specific situations, such as talking on the telephone, everyday life is made up of multichannel nonverbal social interaction.
Typically, many nonverbal cues are available to us when we talk to or observe other people.
How do we use this information?
And how accurately do we use it?
Multichannel Nonverbal Communication
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
In general, women are better at encoding and decoding nonverbal cues.
One exception is that women are less accurate at detecting deception.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
Social role theory of sex differences suggests that this is because women have learned different skills, and one is to be polite and overlook lying.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
According to Alice Eagly’s social role theory, most societies have a division of labor based on gender:
Men work in jobs outside the home.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
According to Alice Eagly’s social role theory, most societies have a division of labor based on gender:
Men work in jobs outside the home.
Women work within the home.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
This division of labor has important consequences.
First, gender-role expectations arise:
Members of the society expect men and women to have certain attributes that are consistent with their role. Thus women are expected to be more nurturing, friendly, expressive, and sensitive than men because of their primary role as caregivers to children and elderly family members.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
This division of labor has important consequences.
Second, men and women develop different sets of skills and attitudes, based on their experiences in their gender roles.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
This division of labor has important consequences.
Second, men and women develop different sets of skills and attitudes, based on their experiences in their gender roles.
Finally, because women are less powerful in many societies and less likely to occupy roles of higher status, it becomes more important for women to learn to be accommodating and polite than it is for men.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication
Implicit Personality Theories:
Filling in the Blanks
To understand other people, we observe their behavior but we also infer their feelings, traits, and motives.
To do so, we use general notions or schemas about which personality traits go together in one person.
Implicit Personality Theory
A type of schema people use to group various kinds of personality traits together; for example, many people believe that someone who is kind is generous as well.
If someone is kind, our implicit personality theory tells us he or she is probably generous as well.
Similarly, we assume that a stingy person is also irritable.
Implicit Personality Theories:
Filling in the Blanks
But relying on schemas can also lead us astray.
We might make the wrong assumptions about an individual.
We might even resort to stereotypical thinking, where our schema, or stereotype, leads us to believe that the individual is like all the other members of his or her group.
Implicit Personality Theories:
Filling in the Blanks
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories
These general notions, or schemas, are shared by people in a culture, and are passed from one generation to another.
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories
A strong implicit personality theory in this culture involves physical attractiveness. We presume that “what is beautiful is good”—that people with physical beauty will also have a whole host of other wonderful qualities.
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories
In China, an implicit personality theory describes a person who embodies traditional Chinese values: creating and maintaining interpersonal harmony, inner harmony, and ren qin (a focus on relationships).
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories
In Western cultures, saying someone has an “artistic personality” implies that the person is creative, intense, and temperamental and has an unconventional lifestyle.
The Chinese, however, do not have a schema or implicit personality theory for an artistic type.
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories
Conversely, in China, there are categories of personality that do not exist in Western cultures.
For example, a shi gú person is someone who is worldly, devoted to his or her family, socially skillful, and somewhat reserved.
Causal Attribution:
Answering the “Why” Question
If an acquaintance says, “It’s great to see you!” does she really mean it?
The point is that even though nonverbal communication is sometimes easy to decode and our implicit personality theories can streamline the way we form impressions, there is still substantial ambiguity as to what a person’s behavior really means.
Perhaps she is acting more thrilled than she really feels, out of politeness.
Perhaps she is outright lying and really can’t stand you.
Causal Attribution:
Answering the “Why” Question
According to attribution theory, we try to determine why people do what they do in order to uncover the feelings and traits that are behind their actions.
This helps us understand and predict our social world.
The Nature of the Attribution Process
Fritz Heider (1958) is frequently referred to as the father of attribution theory.
Heider discussed what he called “naive” or “commonsense” psychology.
In his view, people were like amateur scientists, trying to understand other people’s behavior by piecing together information until they arrived at a reasonable explanation or cause.
Heider was intrigued by what seemed reasonable to people and by how they arrived at their conclusions.
The Nature of the Attribution Process
When trying to decide what causes people’s behavior, we can make one of two attributions:
An internal, dispositional attribution or
An external, situational attribution.
Internal Attribution
The inference that a person is behaving in a certain way because of something about the person, such as attitude, character, or personality.
External Attribution
The inference that a person is behaving a certain way because of something about the situation he or she is in.
The assumption is that most people would respond the same way in that situation.
The Nature of the Attribution Process
Satisfied spouses tend to show one pattern:
Internal attributions for their partners’ positive behaviors (e.g., “She helped me because she’s such a generous person”).
External attributions for their partners’ negative behaviors (e.g., “He said something mean because he’s so stressed at work this week”).
In contrast, spouses in distressed marriages tend to display the opposite pattern:
Their partners’ positive behaviors are chalked up to external causes (e.g., “She helped me because she wanted to impress our friends”).
Negative behaviors are attributed to internal causes (e.g., “He said something mean because he’s a totally self-centered jerk”).
The Nature of the Attribution Process
Although either type of attribution is always possible, Heider (1958) noted that we tend to see the causes of a person’s behavior as residing in that person (internal explanation).
We are perceptually focused on people—they are who we notice.
The situation (the external explanation), which is often hard to see and hard to describe, may be overlooked.
The Covariation Model:
Internal versus External Attributions
Harold Kelley’s major contribution to attribution theory was the idea that we notice and think about more than one piece of information when we form an impression of another person.
Covariation Model
A theory that states that to form an attribution about what caused a person’s behavior, we systematically note the pattern between the presence or absence of possible causal factors and whether or not the behavior occurs.
The Covariation Model:
Internal versus External Attributions
The covariation model focuses on observations of behavior across time, place, actors, and targets.
It examines how the perceiver chooses either an internal or an external attribution.
We make such choices by using information on:
Consensus,
Distinctiveness,
Consistency.
Consensus Information
Information about the extent to which other people behave the same way toward the same stimulus as the actor does.
Distinctiveness Information
Information about the extent to which one particular actor behaves in the same way to different stimuli.
Consistency Information
Information about the extent to which the behavior between one actor and one stimulus is the same across time and circumstances.
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists
Specific errors or biases plague the attribution process.
One common shortcut is the correspondence bias:
the tendency to believe that people’s behavior matches (corresponds to) their dispositions.
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists
The pervasive, fundamental theory or schema most of us have about human behavior is that people do what they do because of the kind of people they are, not because of the situation they are in.
When thinking this way, we are more like personality psychologists, who see behavior as stemming from internal dispositions and traits, than like social psychologists, who focus on the impact of social situations on behavior.
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists
The correspondence bias is so pervasive that many social psychologists call it the fundamental attribution error.
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists
One reason is that when we try to explain someone’s behavior, our focus of attention is usually on the person, not on the surrounding situation.
If we don’t know someone made a F earlier in the day, we can’t use that situational information to help us understand her current behavior.
And even when we know her situation, we still don’t know how she interprets it.
The F may not have upset her if she’s planning to drop the course anyway.
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists
We can’t see the situation, so we ignore its importance.
People, not the situation, have perceptual salience for us.
We pay attention to them, and we tend to think that they alone cause their behavior.
Perceptual Salience
The seeming importance of information that is the focus of people’s attention.
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists
The culprit is one of the mental shortcuts we discussed in chapter 3: the anchoring and adjustment heuristic.
The correspondence bias is another byproduct of this shortcut.
When making attributions, people use the focus of their attention as a starting point.
The Two-Step Process
In sum, we go through a two-step process when we make attributions.
First, we make an internal attribution; we assume that a person’s behavior was due to something about that person.
Then we attempt to adjust this attribution by considering the situation the person was in. But we often don’t make enough of an adjustment in this second step.
Copyright © Education 2010
The Two-Step Process
In sum, we go through a two-step process when we make attributions.
First, we make an internal attribution; we assume that a person’s behavior was due to something about that person.
Then we attempt to adjust this attribution by considering the situation the person was in. But we often don’t make enough of an adjustment in this second step.
Why? Because the first step occurs quickly and spontaneously whereas the second step requires more effort and conscious attention.
The Two-Step Process
We will engage in the second step of attributional processing if we
Consciously slow down and think carefully before reaching a judgment,
Are motivated to reach as accurate a judgment as possible, or
Are suspicious about the behavior of the target person (e.g., we suspect lying).
Copyright © Education 2010
Adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975.
Copyright © Education 2010
Adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975.
Culture and the Correspondence Bias
For decades, it was taken for granted that the correspondence bias was universal:
People everywhere, we thought, applied this cognitive shortcut when forming attributions.
People from individualistic and collectivistic cultures both demonstrate the correspondence bias.
Members of collectivist cultures are more sensitive to situational causes of behavior and more likely to rely on situational explanations, as long as situational variables are salient.
Culture and the Correspondence Bias
North American and some other Western cultures stress individual autonomy. A person is perceived as independent and self-contained; his or her behavior reflects internal traits, motives, and values.
In contrast, East Asian cultures such as those in China, Japan, and Korea stress group autonomy. The individual derives his or her sense of self from the social group to which he or she belongs.
Culture and the Correspondence Bias
It would be a mistake to think that members of collectivist cultures don’t make dispositional attributions.
They do—it’s just a matter of degree.
Culture and the Correspondence Bias
The Actor/Observer Difference
The actor-observer difference is an amplification of the correspondence bias:
We tend to see other people’s behavior as dispositionally caused, while we are more likely to see our own behavior as situationally caused.
The effect occurs because perceptual salience and information availability differ for the actor and the observer.
The Actor/Observer Difference
Actors have more information about themselves than observers do.
Actors know how they’ve behaved over the years; they know what happened to them that morning.
They are far more aware than observers are of both the similarities and the differences in their behavior over time and across situations terms, actors have far more consistency and distinctiveness information about themselves than observers do.
Self-Serving Attributions
Self-Serving Attributions
Explanations for one’s successes that credit internal, dispositional factors and explanations for one’s failures that blame external, situational factors.
Defensive Attributions
Explanations for behavior that avoid feelings of vulnerability and mortality.
Copyright © Education 2010
Self-Serving Attributions
Why do we make self-serving attributions?
Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.
We are particularly likely to engage in self-serving attributions when we fail at something and we feel we can’t improve at it.
The external attribution truly protects our self-esteem, as there is little hope we can do better in the future.
But if we believe we can improve, we’re more likely to attribute our current failure to internal causes and then work on improving.
Self-Serving Attributions
Why do we make self-serving attributions?
Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.
We want people to think well of us and to admire us. Telling others that our poor performance was due to some external cause puts a “good face” on failure; many people call this strategy “making excuses.”
Self-Serving Attributions
Why do we make self-serving attributions?
Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.
We want people to think well of us and to admire us. Telling others that our poor performance was due to some external cause puts a “good face” on failure; many people call this strategy “making excuses.”
We know more about our own efforts than we do about other people’s.
Self-Serving Attributions
One form of defensive attribution is to believe that bad things happen only to bad people or at least, only to people who make stupid mistakes or poor choices.
Therefore, bad things won’t happen to us because we won’t be that stupid or careless.
Melvin Lerner called this the belief in a just world—the assumption that people get what they deserve and deserve what they get.
Self-Serving Attributions
The just world belief has unfortunate consequences:
Victims of crimes or accidents are often seen as causing their own fate.
People tend to believe that rape victims are to blame for the rape.
Battered wives are often seen as responsible for their abusive husbands’ actions.
Culture and Other
Attributional Biases
There is some evidence for cross-cultural differences in the Actor-Observer Effect and in Self-Serving and Defensive Attributions.
Typically, the difference occurs between Western, individualistic cultures and Eastern, collectivistic cultures.
How Accurate Are Our Attributions and Impressions?
Our impressions are sometimes wrong because of the mental shortcuts we use when forming social judgments.
To improve the accuracy of your attributions, remember that the mental shortcuts we use, such as the correspondence bias, can lead us to the wrong conclusions sometimes.
Even with such biases operating, we are quite accurate perceivers of other people.
We do very well most of the time.
In fact, most of us are more accurate than we realize.
How Accurate Are Our Attributions and Impressions?
Our impressions are sometimes wrong because of the mental shortcuts we use when forming social judgments.
To improve the accuracy of your attributions, remember that the mental shortcuts we use, such as the correspondence bias, can lead us to the wrong conclusions sometimes.
Even with such biases operating, we are quite accurate perceivers of other people.
We do very well most of the time.
In fact, most of us are more accurate than we realize.
In short, we are capable of making both stunningly accurate assessments of people and horrific attributional mistakes.
Social Psychology
Elliot Aronson
University of California, Santa Cruz
Timothy D. Wilson
University of Virginia
Robin M. Akert
Wellesley College
slides prepared by
Travis Langley
Henderson State University
7th edition
Consensus Information
Information about the extent to which other people behave the same way toward the same stimulus as the actor does.
Distinctiveness Information
Information about the extent to which one particular actor behaves in the same way to different stimuli.
Consistency Information
Information about the extent to which the behavior between one actor and one stimulus is the same across time and circumstances.
Copyright © 2010 Education. All rights reserved.
Copyright © 2010 Education. All rights reserved.
Adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975.
Copyright © 2010 Education. All rights reserved.
Adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975.
Self-Serving Attributions
Self-Serving Attributions
Explanations for one’s successes that credit internal, dispositional factors and explanations for one’s failures that blame external, situational factors.
Defensive Attributions
Explanations for behavior that avoid feelings of vulnerability and mortality.
Copyright © 2010 Education. All rights reserved.
How Accurate Are Our Attributions and Impressions?
Our impressions are sometimes wrong because of the mental shortcuts we use when forming social judgments.
To improve the accuracy of your attributions, remember that the mental shortcuts we use, such as the correspondence bias, can lead us to the wrong conclusions sometimes.
Even with such biases operating, we are quite accurate perceivers of other people.
We do very well most of the time.
In fact, most of us are more accurate than we realize.
In short, we are capable of making both stunningly accurate assessments of people and horrific attributional mistakes.
Social Psychology
Elliot Aronson
University of California, Santa Cruz
Timothy D. Wilson
University of Virginia
Robin M. Akert
Wellesley College
slides prepared by
Travis Langley
Henderson State University
7th edition
Social Psychology
Elliot Aronson
University of California, Santa Cruz
Timothy D. Wilson
University of Virginia
Robin M. Akert
Wellesley College
slides prepared by
Travis Langley
Henderson State University
7th edition
|
|
Comments (0)
|
Post your homework questions and get free online help from our incredible volunteers
|